Eye-tracking evidence of a relationship between attentional bias for emotional faces and depression severity in patients with treatment-resistant depression (2024)

Eye-tracking evidence of a relationship between attentional bias for emotional faces and depression severity in patients with treatment-resistant depression

Download PDF
Download PDF
  • Article
  • Open access
  • Published:
  • Laetitia Imbert1,
  • Cécilia Neige1,2,
  • Rémi Moirand2,
  • Giulia Piva2,
  • Benoit Bediou3,
  • William Vallet1,2 &
  • Jerome Brunelin1,2

Scientific Reports volume14, Articlenumber:12000 (2024) Cite this article

  • 562 Accesses

  • 3 Altmetric

  • Metrics details

Subjects

  • Depression
  • Emotion

Abstract

In a retrospective study, 54 patients with treatment-resistant major depressive disorder (TRD) completed a free-viewing task in which they had to freely explore pairs of faces (an emotional face (happy or sad) opposite to a neutral face). Attentional bias to emotional faces was calculated for early and sustained attention. We observed a significant negative correlation between depression severity as measured by the 10-item Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) and sustained attention to happy faces. In addition, we observed a positive correlation between depression severity and sustained attention to sad faces. No significant correlation between depression severity and early attention was found for either happy or sad faces. Although conclusions from the current study are limited by the lack of comparison with a control group, the eye-tracking free-viewing task appears to be a relevant, accessible and easy-to-use tool for measuring depression severity through emotional attentional biases in TRD.

Similar content being viewed by others

Eye-tracking evidence of a relationship between attentional bias for emotional faces and depression severity in patients with treatment-resistant depression (1)

Experiences of maltreatment in childhood and attention to facial emotions in healthy young women

Article Open access 12 March 2022

Eye-tracking evidence of a relationship between attentional bias for emotional faces and depression severity in patients with treatment-resistant depression (2)

Paying attention to attention in depression

Article Open access 07 November 2019

Eye-tracking evidence of a relationship between attentional bias for emotional faces and depression severity in patients with treatment-resistant depression (3)

Attentional bias towards negative stimuli in healthy individuals and the effects of trait anxiety

Article Open access 16 July 2020

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is one of the most common mental health disorders, affecting 4.4% of the world’s population1. In addition to the classic clinical symptoms (e.g., feelings of guilt, sadness), MDD is also characterized by cognitive impairments that contribute to the overall burden of the illness. Among these, deficits in emotional processing have attracted a particular interest because of their hypothesized role in the onset and maintenance of depressive symptoms2. Several studies have shown that, compared to healthy controls, people with MDD exhibit a bias toward negative stimuli over positive or neutral information3, and conversely, tend to neglect positive information4.

Interestingly, significant relationships were observed between depression severity and the intensity of emotional biases. One study using different types of stimuli (e.g., emotional words, scenes, and faces) found an association between depression severity and attention to negative information5, while another study using only emotional words found no relationship6, calling for further research on this topic.

Better characterizing attentional biases to emotional stimuli in MDD patients is crucial, as they have been shown to be a stable vulnerability factor for depression2, to persist in remitted depression and to predict future depressive symptomatology7.

It is worth noting that the mentioned studies investigating attentional biases in MDD have primarily used motor reaction time tasks and indirect measures of attention such as the stroop, dot-probe or cueing tasks. However, these tasks primarily assess early attention allocation (direction bias) which is thought to be less impaired in MDD than sustained attention allocation (duration bias)8,9. In addition, reaction time measures used to assess attentional processes have extremely low reliability10 and do not accurately capture emotional processing in patients who have difficulty responding by pressing a keyboard, due to psychom*otor retardation or catatonic symptoms. Eye-tracking can overcome these limitations by providing a direct and objective measure of sustained and early attention through the recording of eye movement.

Free-viewing eye-tracking meta-analysis and review have reported that patients with MDD show a robust attentional bias away from positive stimuli and a less robust bias toward negative stimuli, compared to controls9,11. Importantly, patients with MDD show a more pronounced attentional bias toward negative stimuli than participants with subclinical depression11. Several studies have examined the relationship between depression severity and attentional bias toward emotional faces using eye-tracking in patients with MDD, but have yielded inconsistent conclusions. Some have focused solely on investigating the relationship between symptom severity and sustained attentional bias for both positive and negative information12,13,14 revealing no significant correlation. Conversely, other studies have used a free-viewing task to examine both early and sustained attention, leading to conflicting results. For instance, Duque and Vázquez15 identified a significant correlation only between the depression severity and the sustained attentional bias for sad faces, whereas Bodenschatz et al.16 reported no such correlation.

To address these discrepancies presumed to stem from methodological differences, the present study aims to investigate the relationship between both sustained and early attentional biases for emotional faces (happy and sad) and depression severity in patients with moderate to severe treatment-resistant MDD (TRD) with stable medication for 4weeks. In this study, we hypothesized that there would be inverse correlations between attentional bias to positive and negative emotions and the severity of depression. The ultimate goal of this study is to establish eye tracking in a free-viewing task as a reliable tool for assessing the severity of depression.

Results

Clinical and sociodemographic characteristics

Clinical and sociodemographic characteristics are detailed in Table 1.

Full size table

Relationship between depression severity and sustained emotional attentional bias

A significant negative correlation between depression severity and sustained attention bias for happy faces (pcorr < 0.01; r = -0.40; IC95% = [−0.61; −0.15]) was observed with strong evidence in favor of H1 (BF10 = 14.77; Fig.1A). There was also a significant positive correlation between depression severity and sustained attention bias for sad faces (pcorr < 0.001; r = 0.51; IC95% = [0.28; 0.68]) with decisive evidence in favor of H1 (BF10 = 281.8; Fig.1B). Multiple linear regression analyses showed no effect of age or sex on sustained attentional bias (see Table 2).

Correlation between depression severity and emotional bias for sustained attention. (A) Significant negative correlation between depression severity and sustained attention bias for happy faces (r = −0.40; pcorr < 0.01; BF10 = 14.77). (B) Significant positive correlation between depression severity and sustained attention bias for sad faces (r = 0.51; pcorr < 0.001; BF10 = 281.8).

Full size image

For sustained attentional bias toward happy faces, the multiple linear regression model accounted for 19.7% of the variation in the attentional bias to happy faces and reached statistical significance (F(3,50) = 4.088, p = 0.011; R = 0.444; adj. R square = 0.149). The results show that only the MADRS score was statistically significant in predicting the attentional bias to happy faces while sex and age were not. For sustained attentional bias to sad face, the overall model explained 26.8% of the variation in the attentional bias to sad faces and was significant (F(3,50) = 6.093, p = 0.001; R = 0.517; adj. R square = 0.224). Again, the results show that only the MADRS score was statistically significant in predicting the attentional bias to sad faces while sex and age were not (Table 2).

Relationship between depression severity and early emotional attentional bias

Analyses revealed no significant correlation between the depression severity and the laterality quotient for either happy faces (pcorr = 0.052; r = 0.34; IC95% = [0.08; 0.56]; substantial evidence in favor of H1: BF10 = 6.846, n = 53) or sad faces (pcorr = 0.90; r = -0.17; IC95% = [−0.11; 0.42]; anecdotal evidence in favor of H0: BF10 = 0.35).

Discussion

We aimed to characterize the relationship between depression severity and emotional bias using a straightforward measure of sustained and early attention in patients with TRD.

Regarding sustained attention, the analyses showed strong evidence for a negative correlation between emotional bias to happy faces and the depression severity, and decisive evidence for a positive correlation between emotional bias to sad faces and the depression severity. This suggests that the more severe the depression, the more time participants spent looking at sad faces and the less time they spent looking at happy faces compared to neutral faces. Strengthening this robust association between depression severity and attentional bias, multiple linear regression analyses show that only the MADRS score is a predictor of sustained attentional bias, while gender and age do not contribute significantly to the prediction. Some of these findings are supported by previous eye-tracking studies, which have reported a more pronounced attentional bias in patients diagnosed with MDD compare to participants with subclinical depression11, a positive correlation between depression severity and sustained attentional bias to sad faces15, a longer DTs to sad faces, and a lack of bias to happy faces compared to controls17,18.

Regarding early attentional biases, no significant relationships were observed between the orientation of the first saccade to sad and happy faces and the depression severity. These findings are consistent with several previous eye-tracking studies showing that people with MDD do not show an initial orientation bias to emotional stimuli8,9.

The current study replicates and extends previous findings by showing in the same FVT study that there is an association between sustained emotional bias for both positive and negative stimuli and depression severity, but not with early emotional attentional bias. Although these results are encouraging some limitations that curtail the generalization of the results must be noted. First, our statistical model only controls for the effect of MADRS score, age and sex on cognitive bias, but it is known that other variables could have an impact on this cognitive function, such as treatments or disease characteristics (course of the disease, duration of episode, number of relapses, etc.). Second, it should be noted that results were obtained in patients with TRD, a specific subgroup of patients that may present differences from a clinical19 and neurobiological point of view20. However, the inclusion of treatment-resistant patients with stable antidepressant treatment for at least 4weeks allowed us to control for the potential beneficial effect of medication on emotional processing, including FVT21. Third, the study suffers from lack of a comparison group (either healthy volunteers or people with subclinical depression). Finally, the number of face pairs presented may seem small, but this allows us to propose a task that can be completed quickly and integrated into the patient's examination routine (< 5min). Then, because we only included ‘sad’ and ‘happy’ faces, the generalization of the current conclusions to other emotions is limited.

Finally, using a task that is accessible and easy to integrate into patients' assessment routines, we showed that sustained attentional bias toward sad and away from happy faces increases with depression severity. The FVT may be a useful tool for objective assessment of depression severity in clinical settings.

Material and method

This retrospective study was approved by a local ethics committee (Comité d’Ethique de la REcherche du VInatier—CEREVI, id number #2023/006, on April 24th, 2023), was performed in accordance with relevant French guidelines and regulations using data from an authorized anonymized database (Commission Nationale de l’Informatique et des Libertés CNIL, MR-003-2017-002), and in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants (and/or their legal guardians when applicable) gave written informed consent for participation and for publication of the study. This retrospective study was not registered in a public database prior to its execution.

Sample

We reviewed data from patients who attended our clinical unit for treatment-resistant depression between December 2018 and December 2022. Treatment-resistant depression was defined as a failure to respond to two or more antidepressant regimens despite adequate dose and duration and adherence to treatment22. The 10-item Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS10)23 was used to assess depression severity. It is a clinician-rated questionnaire known for its validity and inter-rater reliability24. It is designed to assess depression severity through 10 items that focuses on the symptoms of depression (e.g., sadness, tension, and pessimistic thoughts).

Patients were eligible if they had a DSM-5 diagnosis of MDD, have moderate to severe depression as indicated by a MADRS10 score ≥ 20 even on stable medication (dosage and molecule) for 4weeks, and have completed the free viewing task (FVT) on the day of their first visit to our clinical unit. Exclusion criteria included neurological (e.g., dementia) or psychiatric comorbidities, unanalyzable eye-tracking data, incomplete clinical information, or withdrawal of consent. Of the 61 eligible patients, 7 patients were not included due to unprocessable eye-tracking data, resulting in a final analyzed sample of 54 participants with moderate to severe depression.

Eye tracking free-viewing task

The FVT consisted of showing pairs of faces: one emotional (happy or sad) and one neutral, presented randomly for 3500ms to the right and left of a fixation cross lasting 1500ms. We presented 23 pairs of stimuli: 11 happy-neutral and 12 sad-neutral pairs, with 12 different identities (6 males, 6 females). The faces were extracted from the Ekman and Friesen open set25. Throughout the task, gaze position and eye movements were recorded with an eye-tracking system (SMI SensoMotoric Instruments with BeGaze 3.6.52, Teltow, Germany).

Eye tracking measurement analysis

Early (direction bias) and sustained (duration bias) attention allocation was assessed per participant based on eye-movements recording26.

Dwell time (DT), the total time (ms) of all fixations on the faces, was used to measure sustained attention allocation (duration bias). The difference between emotional and neutral DTs was used to calculate an emotional bias score.

The orientation of the first saccade, corresponding to the first face the participant looked at, was used to measure early attention allocation (direction bias). A laterality quotient was calculated as the difference between the number of times the participant made a first saccade to the emotional face and the number of times where the first saccades were to the neutral face, divided by the total number of trials.

For both measures, a positive value indicates a bias toward emotional faces, while a negative value indicates a bias toward neutral faces.

Statistical analysis

We used Pearson’s correlations with Bonferroni adjustment (pcorr) to examine the relationship between sustained and early emotional attentional biases and depression severity (MADRS10 scores). To complement the frequentist statistics, we also performed Bayesian analyses using BF10, which provided us with a likelihood ratio of the alternative hypothesis (i.e., H1: correlation between variables) to the null hypothesis (i.e., H0: no correlation). Finally, to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the findings, multiple linear regression analyses were conducted to assess the predictive significance of the MADRS score, age, and sex on sustained attentional bias to happy faces and sustained attentional bias to sad faces. All statistical analyses were performed using Rstudio version 2021.09.2+382 (Richmond Hill, Canada).

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author, [J.B.]. The data are not publicly available due to local restrictions (e.g., their containing information that could compromise the privacy of research participants).

References

  1. WHO. Depression and Other Common Mental Disorders: Global Health Estimates (Springer, 2017).

  2. Gotlib, I. H. & Joormann, J. Cognition and depression: Current status and future directions. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 6, 285–312 (2010).

    Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

  3. Leyman, L., De-Raedt, R., Schacht, R. & Koster, E. H. W. Attentional biases for angry faces in unipolar depression. Psychol. Med. 37, 393 (2007).

    Article PubMed Google Scholar

  4. Matthews, G. R. & Antes, J. R. Visual attention and depression: Cognitive biases in the eye fixations of the dysphoric and the nondepressed. Cogn. Ther. Res. 16, 359–371 (1992).

    Article Google Scholar

  5. Baert, S., De Raedt, R. & Koster, E. H. W. Depression-related attentional bias: The influence of symptom severity and symptom specificity. Cogn. Emot. 24, 1044–1052 (2010).

    Article Google Scholar

  6. Krings, A., Heeren, A., Fontaine, P. & Blairy, S. Attentional biases in depression: Relation to disorder severity, rumination, and anhedonia. Compr. Psychiatry 100, 152173 (2020).

    Article PubMed Google Scholar

  7. Shamai-Leshem, D., Linetzky, M. & Bar-Haim, Y. Attention biases in previously depressed individuals: A meta-analysis and implications for depression recurrence. Cogn. Ther. Res. 46, 1033–1048 (2022).

    Article Google Scholar

  8. Caseras, X., Garner, M., Bradley, B. P. & Mogg, K. Biases in visual orienting to negative and positive scenes in dysphoria: An eye movement study. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116, 491–497 (2007).

    Article PubMed Google Scholar

  9. Suslow, T., Hußlack, A., Kersting, A. & Bodenschatz, C. M. Attentional biases to emotional information in clinical depression: A systematic and meta-analytic review of eye tracking findings. J. Affect. Disord. 274, 632–642 (2020).

    Article PubMed Google Scholar

  10. Carlson, J. M., Fang, L., Coughtry-Carpenter, C. & Foley, J. Reliability of attention bias and attention bias variability to climate change images in the dot-probe task. Front. Psychol. 13, 1021858 (2023).

    Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

  11. Armstrong, T. & Olatunji, B. O. Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: A meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 704–723 (2012).

    Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

  12. Disner, S. G., Shumake, J. D. & Beevers, C. G. Self-referential schemas and attentional bias predict severity and naturalistic course of depression symptoms. Cogn. Emot. 31, 632–644 (2017).

    Article PubMed Google Scholar

  13. Klawohn, J. et al. Aberrant attentional bias to sad faces in depression and the role of stressful life events: Evidence from an eye-tracking paradigm. Behav. Res. Ther. 135, 103762 (2020).

    Article PubMed Google Scholar

  14. Woolridge, S. M., Harrison, G. W., Best, M. W. & Bowie, C. R. Attention bias modification in depression: A randomized trial using a novel, reward-based, eye-tracking approach. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 71, 101621 (2021).

    Article PubMed Google Scholar

  15. Duque, A. & Vázquez, C. Double attention bias for positive and negative emotional faces in clinical depression: Evidence from an eye-tracking study. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 46, 107–114 (2015).

    Article PubMed Google Scholar

  16. Bodenschatz, C. M., Skopinceva, M., Ruß, T. & Suslow, T. Attentional bias and childhood maltreatment in clinical depression—an eye-tracking study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 112, 83–88 (2019).

    Article PubMed Google Scholar

  17. Lazarov, A., Ben-Zion, Z., Shamai, D., Pine, D. S. & Bar-Haim, Y. Free viewing of sad and happy faces in depression: A potential target for attention bias modification. J. Affect. Disord. 238, 94–100 (2018).

    Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

  18. Lu, S. et al. Attentional bias scores in patients with depression and effects of age: A controlled, eye-tracking study. J. Int. Med. Res. 45, 1518–1527 (2017).

    Article ADS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

  19. Kornstein, S. G. & Schneider, R. K. Clinical features of treatment-resistant depression. J. Clin. Psychiatry 62, 18–25 (2001).

    CAS PubMed Google Scholar

  20. Runia, N. et al. The neurobiology of treatment-resistant depression: A systematic review of neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 132, 433–448 (2022).

    Article PubMed Google Scholar

  21. Wells, T. T., Clerkin, E. M., Ellis, A. J. & Beevers, C. G. Effect of antidepressant medication use on emotional information processing in major depression. AJP 171, 195–200 (2014).

    Article Google Scholar

  22. McIntyre, R. S. et al. Treatment-resistant depression: Definition, prevalence, detection, management, and investigational interventions. World Psychiatry 22, 394–412 (2023).

    Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

  23. Montgomery, S. A. & Åsberg, M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br. J. Psychiatry 134, 382–389 (1979).

    Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

  24. Davidson, J., Turnbull, C. D., Strickland, R., Miller, R. & Graves, K. The Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Scale: Reliability and validity. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 73, 544–548 (1986).

    Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

  25. Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. Pictures of Facial Affect (Consulting Psychologists Press, 1976).

    Google Scholar

  26. Werthmann, J., Jansen, A. & Roefs, A. Make up your mind about food: A healthy mindset attenuates attention for high-calorie food in restrained eaters. Appetite 105, 53–59 (2016).

    Article PubMed Google Scholar

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the study nurses of the PSYR2 team and the Ugo Cerletti Unit. This research was funded by the joint Scientific council of CH Le Vinatier with the University Lumière Lyon 2 (#CSLV 09, RM).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, CNRS, INSERM, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon CRNL U1028 UMR5292, PSYR2, 69500, Bron, France

    Laetitia Imbert,Cécilia Neige,William Vallet&Jerome Brunelin

  2. Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier, Bat 416 1st floor, 95 boulevard Pinel, 696500, Bron, France

    Cécilia Neige,Rémi Moirand,Giulia Piva,William Vallet&Jerome Brunelin

  3. Faculté de Psychologie et Sciences de l’Education Campus Biotech, Université de Genève, 9 chemin des mines, 1211, Geneva, Switzerland

    Benoit Bediou

Authors

  1. Laetitia Imbert

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  2. Cécilia Neige

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  3. Rémi Moirand

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  4. Giulia Piva

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  5. Benoit Bediou

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  6. William Vallet

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  7. Jerome Brunelin

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

Contributions

L.I., C.N. & J.B. wrote the first draft of the manuscript. W.V. & B.B. helped with the statistical analysis. G.P. & R.M. performed the clinical investigation and assessment. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jerome Brunelin.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Eye-tracking evidence of a relationship between attentional bias for emotional faces and depression severity in patients with treatment-resistant depression (5)

Cite this article

Imbert, L., Neige, C., Moirand, R. et al. Eye-tracking evidence of a relationship between attentional bias for emotional faces and depression severity in patients with treatment-resistant depression. Sci Rep 14, 12000 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-62251-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-62251-4

Keyword

  • Major depression
  • Facial emotion
  • Free-viewing task
  • Emotional bias

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Eye-tracking evidence of a relationship between attentional bias for emotional faces and depression severity in patients with treatment-resistant depression (2024)
Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Amb. Frankie Simonis

Last Updated:

Views: 6236

Rating: 4.6 / 5 (56 voted)

Reviews: 95% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Amb. Frankie Simonis

Birthday: 1998-02-19

Address: 64841 Delmar Isle, North Wiley, OR 74073

Phone: +17844167847676

Job: Forward IT Agent

Hobby: LARPing, Kitesurfing, Sewing, Digital arts, Sand art, Gardening, Dance

Introduction: My name is Amb. Frankie Simonis, I am a hilarious, enchanting, energetic, cooperative, innocent, cute, joyous person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.